Home Article
International Journal of Healthcare Simulation
image
Exploring facilitator gaze patterns during difficult debriefing through eye-tracking analysis: a pilot study

DOI:10.54531/pvrt9874, Pages: 1-7
Article Type: Original Research, Article History
Abstract

Background

Managing difficult debriefing can be challenging for simulation facilitators. Debriefers may use eye contact as a strategy to build and maintain psychological safety during debriefing. Visual dominance ratio (VDR), a measure of social power, is defined as the percentage of time making eye contact while speaking divided by the percentage of time making eye contact while listening. Little is known about eye gaze patterns during difficult debriefings.

Aim

To demonstrate the feasibility of examining eye gaze patterns (i.e. VDR) among junior and senior facilitators during difficult debriefing.

Methods

We recruited 10 trained simulation facilitators (four seniors and six juniors) and observed them debriefing two actors. The actors were scripted to play the role of learners who were engaged in the first scenario, followed by upset (emotional) and confrontational in the second and third scenarios, respectively. The participant facilitators wore an eye-tracking device to record their eye movements and fixation duration. The fixation durations and VDRs were calculated and summarized with median and interquartile range. We explore the effect of scenarios and training level on VDRs using Friedman tests and Wilcoxon rank sum tests.

Results

All 10 participants completed all three scenarios. There were no statistically significant differences in VDRs between the junior and senior facilitators for all three scenarios (baseline: p = 0.17; confrontational: p = 0.76; and emotional: p = 0.61). The VDR did not change significantly between scenarios among junior (p = 0.85) and senior facilitators (p = 0.78). The senior group showed higher variability in VDR than the junior group.

Conclusion

The use of eye-tracking device to measure VDR during debriefings is feasible. We did not demonstrate a difference between junior and seniors in eye gaze patterns during difficult debriefings.

Wilkie, Roze des Ordons, Cheng, and Lin: Exploring facilitator gaze patterns during difficult debriefing through eye-tracking analysis: a pilot study

What this study adds

  • The use of eye-tracking techniques has focused mainly on learners previously; however, it can also be used to explore the behaviour of simulation facilitators.
  • Gaze patterns of a simulation facilitator during debriefing are measurable.
  • Visual dominance ratio (VDR) can be calculated using eye-tracking techniques as a surrogate of social power during debriefing.
  • Some simulation facilitators may be purposefully adapting gaze patterns based on the response of the learners during difficult debriefing.

Introduction

Debriefing in healthcare simulation may be challenging due to factors associated with the learners, the facilitators, the simulation environment and/or other external variables [1]. Learner-related factors that influence the nature of conversations during debriefing include inherent affect, personality, previous experiences or personal biases [2,3]. The degree of learner engagement, interest and emotion influences the process and outcomes of debriefing [4].

Poorly facilitated debriefings may compromise psychological safety and result in missed learning opportunities. Facilitators may use verbal and non-verbal communication strategies (e.g. eye contact, body language) to build and maintain psychological safety during debriefing [4]. Non-verbal communication accounts for a large proportion of the meaning conveyed in adult conversation [5]. Consequently, being cognizant of gaze has been suggested as both a proactive and reactive strategy to implement when encountering difficulties in debriefing [4]. However, many facilitators lack the knowledge, skills and awareness to effectively modulate eye contact during these challenging conversations.

Eye-tracking devices can be used as an objective tool to measure, assess and provide feedback on gaze pattern in simulation [6]. Currently, there is a paucity of research assessing gaze patterns during debriefing, with prior studies [6] focusing primarily on the learners rather than the facilitators. Attentional behaviour can be evaluated by measuring an individual’s eye movements [6]. Specifically, the visual dominance ratio (VDR) is a quotient of the percentage of time making eye contact while speaking divided by the percentage of time making eye contact while listening [79]. Finding the appropriate amount of eye contact is important – too much eye contact may instinctively feel rude, hostile or condescending in some situations, whereas in other contexts, longer eye contact may facilitate rapport and trust [10]. Successful interaction in a social context requires modulation of social behaviour [10]. Although modulating gaze has been described as a debriefing technique [4,11], quantitative data describing VDR during debriefing have not previously been reported.

The purpose of this pilot study was to determine the feasibility of using eye tracking during debriefing to capture facilitator gaze patterns, and to describe the VDR of facilitators in different debriefing situations. Secondarily, we aimed to explore gaze patterns among junior and senior facilitators when encountering challenges in debriefing.

Methods

This was a descriptive pilot study designed to describe the variations in facilitators’ gaze patterns in two difficult debriefing situations. A qualitative analysis of interview data obtained from the same participants has been published elsewhere [11]. Research ethics board approval was obtained from the Conjoint Health Research Ethics Board at University of Calgary (CHREB; REB 19-0100).

Participants

This study was conducted at Alberta Children’s Hospital KidSIM Simulation Center. We recruited nurses and physicians who received formal simulation debriefing training (i.e. the KidSIM Foundations debriefing course) within the preceding years. The course teaches the Promoting Excellence and Reflective Learning in Simulation (PEARLS) blended-method approach to debriefing [12,13]. Participants were considered not eligible for the study if they were unable to wear the eye-tracking device or if they had severe visual impairment. All participants provided informed consent. We enrolled a convenience sample of 10 participants for this pilot study. The sample size estimation is not applicable in this study, as it is a descriptive study that doesn’t have any hypotheses.

Scenario design

Participants were asked to complete a demographic characteristics survey. They then watched a video of a simulated cardiac arrest scenario, which depicted a group of learners participating in a simulated adult cardiac arrest from ventricular fibrillation, involving several individual and teamwork performance gaps, such as the lack of closed-loop communication, delayed defibrillation and interruption of cardiopulmonary resuscitation. The participant then took the role of simulation facilitator and was asked to debrief the same two actors who were playing the role of the learners from the video. The actors were instructed to acted as their respective roles as physicians, respiratory therapists and nurses.

All participant facilitators were prebriefed in a standardized fashion before watching the video. Before debriefing, the participants were asked to wear an eye-tracking device (Tobii Pro Glasses™) with corrective lenses if required. We allowed sufficient time for the participants to familiarize themselves with the equipment and the environment. The participants then debriefed the same actors portrayed in three different scenarios:

  • Debriefing scenario 1 – Baseline: The actors portrayed engaged, insightful, responsive and cooperative learners during the debriefing.
  • Debriefing scenario 2 – Emotional: One actor portrayed a learner who was upset about their performance, tearful, self-deprecating and unable to focus on the debriefing. The second actor portrayed a learner who attempted to reassure the other learner.
  • Debriefing scenario 3 – Confrontational: The actors portrayed two learners who were confrontational, argumentative and defensive about issues that arose during the scenario.

The second and third debriefing scenarios were developed based on difficult learner-specific interactions that have been identified in the debriefing literature [4,14]. Participants did three consecutive debriefings of the clinical case depicted in the video, with a brief pause and reset between scenarios. They had 10 minutes to debrief scenario 1 and 20 minutes to debrief each of the other two scenarios due to their complexity. Scenario 1 was used to capture the participants’ baseline gaze pattern. The order of scenario 2 and scenario 3 was randomized using a random number generator, with half of the participants debriefing scenario 2 then 3, and the other half in the reverse order. Participants were instructed to debrief using their usual approach and techniques without the need to complete the debriefing, and that debriefing would end at the allotted time regardless of their progress in debriefing.

The actors playing the learner roles in the scenarios were trained to portray the characteristics described above. Training involved practicing the debriefing scenarios, reviewing ways to demonstrate emotional distress and frustration, as well as discussing how to respond to participants’ corresponding behaviours. The actors consistently portrayed their role and associated emotions for each participant. To ensure consistency, the learner roles were played by the same research team members. All sessions were observed, timed and recorded by one research team member. All debriefing sessions were video-recorded through the eye-tracking device and two video cameras (overhead and behind the facilitator views) in the simulation centre.

Outcome measures

The eye-tracking data were analysed using the Tobii Pro Lab™ software (Tobii, Sweden, version 1.114), which captures eyes movement and measures fixation time. Fixation time was defined as eyes moving at a velocity of less than 30 degrees per second. We defined the faces of the learners as area of interest (AOI), and the software measures the duration when participant facilitators have ‘eye contact’ with the actor learner’s face. We did not use actors’ eyes as AOI, as they are too small an area to be accurately captured. By manually screen the video to identify time when the participant facilitator was speaking and listening to the actor learners, we obtained all elements to calculate VDR.

Our primary outcome measure was the VDR, calculated as a quotient of the percentage of time looking while speaking and the percentage of time looking while listening [15] for each debriefing.

VDR=Timeoflookingwhilespeaking/TotalspeakingtimeTimeoflookingwhilelistening/Totallisteningtime=%oftimelookingwhilespeaking%oftimelookingwhilelistening

Our secondary outcome measures were: (1) percentage of time looking while speaking; and (2) percentage of time looking while listening. In comparing junior to senior facilitators, we defined the senior facilitators as having greater than 5 years of debriefing experience and having taken the advanced debriefing courses involving content like difficult debriefing, co-debriefing or peer coaching. The junior facilitators were those who did not meet these criteria.

Statistical analysis

Demographic characteristics were summarized with descriptive statistics (median and interquartile for numeric variable; count and percentage for categorical variables). The first 5 minutes of data were analysed for each debriefing since it was difficult for the actors to maintain emotional or confrontational for a long time. Given the nature of the data (e.g. small sample size and non-normal distribution), non-parametric tests were conducted for all analyses. The percentage of time participant facilitators looking at the learners when listening versus when speaking was compared with Wilcoxon signed rank tests. The differences between three scenarios were compared with Friedman tests, and Kendall’s W was presented as effect size. Wilcoxon rank sum tests were used to detect differences between junior and senior groups.

Results

A total of 10 participants took part in the study, four of whom were senior facilitators (Supplementary material 1). A total of 30 videos with eye-tracking data were included in the analyses. Due to technical issues, two of the videos (one confrontational scenario and one emotional scenario) were incomplete, as each had less than 5 minutes of eye-tracking data recorded. We analysed the data from all videos, including the data captured from the two incomplete videos. Gaze samples is the percentage of gaze data correctly identified and measured, which ranged from 74% to 98% (mean 89.6%) in this study. An example of an eye-tracking plot for a participant interacting with learner actors is presented in Figure 1.

Example of eye-tracking plot for a participant facilitator and learner actors. Each spot represents a single instance of a facilitators’ eye fixation. A fixation is defined as eyes moving at a velocity of less than 30 degrees per second. Each spot may represent a different duration of fixation.
Figure 1:

Example of eye-tracking plot for a participant facilitator and learner actors. Each spot represents a single instance of a facilitators’ eye fixation. A fixation is defined as eyes moving at a velocity of less than 30 degrees per second. Each spot may represent a different duration of fixation.

Visual dominance ratio

Participants showed an overall low VDR across scenarios. Although senior facilitators had a slightly higher VDR than the junior facilitators, there were no statistically significant differences between the groups for all three scenarios (baseline: p = 0.17; confrontational: p = 0.76; and emotional: p = 0.61) (Table 1; Figure 2). The VDR did not change significantly between scenarios in both junior (p = 0.85) and senior facilitators (p = 0.78) (Table 1; Figure 3). The senior facilitators have a higher variability in VDR compared with the junior facilitators (Kendall’s W : junior 0.028 vs. senior 0.063, Figure 3).

Table 1:
Visual dominance ratio (VDR). For further calculation details, see text.
Median (IQR) Scenario p -value* p -value** Friedman’s test
p-value
Effect size
Kendal’s W
Baseline Confrontational Emotional
Debriefer Junior (n = 6) 0.67 (0.62–0.78) 0.67 (0.55–1.13) 0.67 (0.61–0.85) 0.60 0.46 0.85 0.028
Senior (n = 4) 0.86 (0.68–1.14) 0.77 (0.53–1.34) 0.81 (0.51–1.62) 0.72 >0.99 0.78 0.063
Total (n = 10) 0.70 (0.65–0.98) 0.67 (0.55–1.10) 0.69 (0.61–0.96) 0.80 0.80 0.91 0.01
p -value*** 0.17 0.76 0.61

IQR: interquartile range.

* Comparison between baseline and confrontational scenarios.

** Comparison between baseline and emotional scenarios.

*** Comparison between junior and senior facilitators.

Visual dominance ratio for baseline, confrontational and emotional learners.
Figure 2:

Visual dominance ratio for baseline, confrontational and emotional learners.

Visual dominance ratio in senior and junior facilitators.
Figure 3:

Visual dominance ratio in senior and junior facilitators.

Percentage of fixation time when listening and speaking during debriefing

The percentage of time looking at learners during listening was higher than that during speaking in all three scenarios (median percentage of time looking when listening vs. when speaking: baseline 58.77% vs. 52.63%; confrontational 68.47% vs. 50.25%; emotional 65.88% vs. 47.10%), although only the difference in the baseline scenario yielded a statistically significant result (baseline: p = 0.017; confrontational: p = 0.11; emotional: p = 0.06) (Table 2; Figure 4). The practice between junior and senior group in percentage of time looking at learners was not statistically significant (Supplementary material 2).

Table 2:
Percentage fixation time on the learners during the first 5 minutes of debriefing. For further details, see text.
Scenario p -value* p -value** Friedman’s test
p-value
Kendal’s W
Median (IQR) Baseline Confrontational Emotional
When listening 58.77 (50.40–81.45) 68.47 (46.90–77.71) 65.88 (40.23–76.69) 0.386 0.445 0.301 0.12
When speaking 52.63 (40.77–56.98) 50.25 (38.90–63.97) 47.10 (35.71–61.15) 0.799 0.959 0.905 0.01
p -value*** 0.017 0.114 0.059

IQR: interquartile range.

* Comparison between baseline and confrontational scenarios.

** Comparison between baseline and emotional scenarios.

*** Comparison between listening and speaking phases.

Fixation time for baseline, confrontational and emotional learners.
Figure 4:

Fixation time for baseline, confrontational and emotional learners.

Discussion

Our study demonstrates that both junior and senior facilitators generally direct their visual gaze more towards learners when listening than speaking, resulting in an overall low VDR during debriefing. The VDRs in confrontational and emotional scenarios were slightly less than the baseline scenarios, although the difference was not statistically significant. We did not identify a significant difference in the VDRs between the junior and senior facilitators, but the senior facilitators had a slightly higher VDR than the junior. We observed an increased variability in the VDR among the senior, whereas junior facilitators had limited changes in the VDR. Below, we explore how these findings contribute to our understanding of how eye contact influences the dynamics of conversation during debriefing.

Eye-tracking technology has been applied to explore and support various aspects of healthcare provider training. A systematic review of 33 eye-tracking technology studies described how eye tracking can be used to support clinical learning, assessment and feedback in medical training [6]. Some of the studies in the review showed how the visual gaze behaviour of the learners changed along a learning curve, and eye tracking was also used to provide feedback and assessment for proficiency. However, these studies have focused on the use of eye-tracking technology in learners. To our knowledge, this is the first study to use eye-tracking devices to measure gaze fixation patterns of simulation facilitators during debriefing to explore the interaction between facilitators and learners.

The primary outcome measure, VDR, has been identified as a meaningful measure of communication and social relations [16]. Visual dominant behaviour can be influenced by personality, status, expertise, gender and team dynamics [15]. The VDR reflects the relative position on the social dominance hierarchy between the conversation partners. Those with a higher social power or more expertise typically displays a VDR close to 1 or even higher [4]. In our study, most facilitators scored a VDR less than 0.8, which may suggest their attempts to maintain psychological safety during debriefing through variation in eye contact. The senior facilitators exhibited slightly higher social dominance during debriefing, as demonstrated by higher VDRs in all three scenarios. There are many possible reasons for this observation, including but not limited to greater experience, higher standing within social hierarchy or more expertise.

The two difficult debriefing scenarios in our study (i.e. confrontational learners and emotional learners) were designed to represent breaches in psychological safety during debriefing. In such difficult debriefing situations, eye contact can be used as both a proactive and a reactive strategy. When noticing a learner is upset during debriefing, one could consider de-escalating the situation by maintaining eye contact when listening while deliberately breaking eye contact while speaking (low VDR) [4]. In our study, we found that junior facilitators failed to change their gaze patterns across the three scenarios (same median VDR in three scenarios). The senior facilitators, although maintaining a slightly higher VDR than the junior, had a lower VDR in the two difficult debriefing scenarios (median VDR: baseline 0.86 vs. confrontational 0.77 and emotional 0.81). This phenomenon suggests that some senior facilitators may be purposefully adapting fixation patterns based on learner response. This could be partly explained by the cognitive load theory. The difficult debriefing scenario imposed a high extraneous load on the junior facilitators, whose intrinsic loads were already high. The extra cognitive load made it difficult for them to have sufficient working memories to process reactive responses. Multiple other factors could potentially influence gaze patterns, such as gender [17], cultural background [18], cognitive overload [19], observation bias and facilitator preference of utilizing pen and paper during debriefing.

Variations in eye contact may or may not achieve its intended impact, as non-verbal communication is complex, dynamic and interconnected, where changes in one dimension affect other dimensions [20]. In addition, facilitators may use strategies other than eye contact to address difficulties that arise during debriefing, such as conversational techniques like validation and normalization [4,14]. This may be another reason explains why there was no significant difference noted in VDRs between junior and seniors.

Cultural background can influence debriefing practices [2123] and gaze patterns. In North America, making good eye contact usually signifies that one is engaged and interested in the conversation, while avoiding eye contact may be interpreted as shyness or lack of self-confidence. However, in some East Asian countries, avoiding eye contact is a sign of politeness and respect, and is culturally appropriate. In some Middle Eastern cultures, there are strict rules around eye contact between the sexes, which is connected to religious laws. Different cultural backgrounds also result in different perceptions on social power and the tenets of psychological safety during debriefing. Like most of the debriefing literature, which affirms Western-centric debriefing style, our study was conducted in a Canadian centre. Extrapolating our conclusions to different cultures may not be appropriate. Future studies should examine the gaze patterns of debriefers from different cultures and how this impacts learners from cultures which differ from that of the debriefer.

Our study has several limitations. First, this research is a pilot study with a limited sample size, therefore, failing to achieve sufficient statistical power to detect a significant difference. However, the data reported in this study have built a foundation for future more strictly designed studies. Second, the project was conducted in an institute with a well-established simulation training program, which was heavily involved in simulation-based research. Most participants adhered to one framework of debriefing (i.e. PEARLS). This may limit the generalizability of the results. Third, we did not standardize the emotional change of the learner actors in each scenario. Instead, the emotion of the learner during debriefing was responsive to the facilitator. For example, if the facilitator used strategies to address the confrontational learner, the learner’s emotions de-escalated in response. We designed the study this way to reflect the circular nature of interpersonal interactions. To minimize potential biases, we decided a priori to analyse the first 5 minutes of gaze data for each participant to capture the most accurate gaze pattern coinciding with the emotional state of the learner in each scenario.

Although this study was small and did not show statistically significant differences, the use of eye-tracking technology in facilitators has opened a new pathway for simulation debriefing research. Attention to gaze patterns may be considered an advanced debriefing skill, and therefore may be more important for debriefers in the maturity phase (experts with deep understand of all concepts applied in contexts) than the debriefers in the discovery phase (i.e. novice debriefer with basic knowledge of key concepts, but little context reference) [24]. Debriefers may not be aware of their gaze patterns, and eye-tracking data could also be used as a source of feedback in debriefer faculty development. Future research could evaluate the relationship between eye-tracking data and quality of debriefing or debriefer cognitive load. Additional efforts might explore how eye-contact patterns change in response to an emotional burst of the learners (e.g. a learner suddenly cries during debriefing).

Conclusion

The use of eye-tracking devices to calculate VDR is a feasible approach to explore ways in which facilitators adapt non-verbal communication to challenges encountered in simulation debriefing. In our examination of the first 5 minutes of different difficult debriefings, we did not demonstrate a significant difference in VDR between junior and senior facilitators. More work is required to explore the association between eye gaze pattern and quality of debriefing.

Supplementary material

Supplementary data are available at The International Journal of Healthcare Simulation online.

Declarations

Acknowledgements

Jennifer Davidson, R.N. Research Coordinator and Simulation Educator at KidSIM Simulation Program, Alberta Children’s Hospital, University of Calgary. Vincent Grant, M.D. Associate Professor, Cumming School of Medicine, University of Calgary. Department of Pediatrics; Department of Emergency Medicine.

Authors’ contributions

All authors participated in the design, editing and writing of the study. All authors approved the final manuscript. RW, ALR, AC and YL designed the study. RW extracted the data, interpreted the results and draft the manuscript. ALR and AC assisted with content and revision. YL conducted the statistical analysis of the data, interpreted the results, created the figures and revised the manuscript.

Funding

None declared.

Availability of data and materials

Raw data were available upon reasonable request.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Research ethics board approval was obtained from the Conjoint Health Research Ethics Board at University of Calgary (CHREB; REB 19-0100). Written consent was obtained from each participant.

Competing interests

AC is a faculty for the Debriefing Academy, which provides debriefing courses for clinical educators.

References

1. 

Steinert Y. The “problem” learner: whose problem is it? AMEE Guide No. 76. Medical Teacher. 2013 Mar 15;35(4):e1035e1045.

2. 

Tiberius RG. Small group teaching a trouble-shooting guide. New York: Routledge. 1999.

3. 

McCrorie P. Teaching and leading small groups. In: Swanwick T, editor. Understanding medical education. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Blackwell. 2013. p. 123136.

4. 

Grant VJ, Robinson T, Catena H, Eppich W, Cheng A. Difficult debriefing situations: a toolbox for simulation educators. Medical Teacher. 2018 May 23;40(7):703712.

5. 

Macdonald E. Difficult conversations in medicine. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 2004.

6. 

Ashraf H, Sodergren MH, Merali N, Mylonas G, Singh H, Darzi A. Eye-tracking technology in medical education: a systematic review. Medical Teacher. 2018 Jan;40(1):6269.

7. 

Dovidio JF, Ellyson SL. Decoding visual dominance: attributions of power based on relative percentages of looking while speaking and looking while listening. Social Psychology Quarterly. 1982 Jun;45(2):106113.

8. 

Koch SC, Baehne CG, Kruse L, Zimmermann F, Zumbach J. Visual dominance and visual egalitarianism: individual and group-level influences of sex and status in group interactions. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior. 2010 Apr 21;34(3):137153.

9. 

Mlodinow L. Subliminal: how your unconscious mind rules your behavior. New York: Random House. 2012.

10. 

Huang J. Fostering learner autonomy within constraints: negotiation and mediation in an atmosphere of collegiality. Prospect. 2006 Dec;21(3):3857.

11. 

Roze des Ordons AL, Cheng A, Lockyer J, Wilkie RD, Grant V, Eppich W. Approaches to interpersonal conflict in simulation debriefings: a qualitative study. Medical Education. 2021 Nov;55(11):12841296.

12. 

Eppich W, Cheng A. Promoting Excellence and Reflective Learning in Simulation (PEARLS): development and rationale for a blended approach to health care simulation debriefing. Simulation in Healthcare. 2015 Apr;10(2):106115.

13. 

Sawyer T, Eppich W, Brett-Fleegler M, Grant V, Cheng A. More than one way to debrief: a critical review of healthcare simulation debriefing methods. Simulation in Healthcare. 2016 Jun;11(3):209217.

14. 

Kolbe M, Eppich W, Rudolph J, et al. Managing psychological safety in debriefings: a dynamic balancing act. BMJ Simulation and Technology Enhanced Learning. 2020 Apr 20;6(3):164171.

15. 

Jamnik J, Žvelc G. The embodiment of power and visual dominance behaviour. Interdisciplinary Description of Complex Systems. 2017 Dec;15(4):228241.

16. 

Jongerius C, Hessels RS, Romijn JA, Smets EMA, Hillen MA. The measurement of eye contact in human interactions: a scoping review. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior. 2020 Apr 20;44(3):363389.

17. 

Lanthier SN, Jarick M, Zhu MJH, Byun CSJ, Kingstone A. Socially communicative eye contact and gender affect memory. Frontiers in Psychology. 2019 May 22;10:1128.

18. 

Akechi H, Senju A, Uibo H, Kikuchi Y, Hasegawa T, Hietanen JK. Attention to eye contact in the West and East: autonomic responses and evaluative ratings. PLoS One. 2013 Mar 13;8(3):e59312.

19. 

Kajimura S, Nomura M. When we cannot speak: eye contact disrupts resources available to cognitive control processes during verb generation. Cognition. 2016 Oct 15;157:352357.

20. 

Argyle M, Dean J. Eye-contact, distance and affiliation. Sociometry. 1965 Sep;28(3):289304.

21. 

Chung HS, Dieckmann P, Issenberg SB. It is time to consider cultural differences in debriefing. Simulation in Healthcare. 2013 Jun;8(3):166170.

22. 

Palaganas JC, Chan AKM, Leighton K. Cultural considerations in debriefing. Simulation in Healthcare. 2021 Dec 1;16(6):407413.

23. 

Ulmer FF, Sharara-Chami R, Lakissian Z, Stocker M, Scott E, Dieckmann P. Cultural prototypes and differences in simulation debriefing. Simulation in Healthcare. 2018 Aug;13(4):239246.

24. 

Cheng A, Eppich W, Kolbe M, Meguerdichian M, Bajaj K, Grant V. A conceptual framework for the development of debriefing skills: a journey of discovery, growth, and maturity. Simulation in Healthcare. 2020 Feb;15(1):5560.